II Manchester Talk May 31, 2012
The study of how
we know (epistemology) has been a fashionable area of scholarship for
centuries, but the fact that it has attracted so much attention is a sign not
of progress, but rather an indicator of regressive decline. It is evidence of a
desperate striving to discover what has caused the elision of our humanity, a
deficit that has enlarged to the point that contemporary scholars no longer
have the tools essential to interpret many ancient, late antique, medieval and
some modern texts that are key to their research. The post-Cartesian mindset
that allows for only one, merely linear epistemology is an existential
anachronism, especially when applied to texts that are based on two
epistemologies—one of them global or holographic—that are interdependent. The grip of the
Cartesian vis [c]e is so strong that many of those who write, who depend and
draw on the holographic part of the mind that is out of their sight and out of
their control—though not out of their influence—cannot seem to make the
connection that they are drawing on a greater, if differently rational,
thinking mind. The concern for the Other needs to begin with one's one mind!
Even those
thinkers who for centuries have sensed that there is something wrong with the
Cartesian project seem unable to grant that the holographic part of the mind
that is not directly accessible is in fact thinking and rational, in fact, far more rational than the self-conscious mind. This
reluctance persists due in part to the lingering influence of Dr Freud and his
nemesis, positivism; and even in the face of Gödel's theorems that prove that
formal systems are both incomplete and self-subverting. Iain McGilchrist likens the present
confusion to a flock of penguins crowding at the edge of an ice cliff, waiting
to see who will be the first to jump into the sea.
The left side of
the diagram indicates the small capacity, linear, hierarchical,
two-dimensional, self-conscious mind that interprets, categorizes and speaks.
David Brooks in The Social Animal
suggests that it can hold in play perhaps 40 items at any one time and it
deludes itself that it has everything under control. This is the self-conscious
mind, as in, 'Don't be so self-conscious; be yourself.' Its world is
artificial; everything it thinks is reified and bent to its own purposes. It
re-presents; its re-presentations are dead. It gives the illusion of
objectivity.
By contrast, the right side
of the diagram indicates what I shall call the deep mind, which is holographic,
and has an almost unfathomable capacity. It is objective in fact. It can hold
up to 11 million items in play at any one
time. This part of the mind sees directly; the world manifests to this part of the mind. It processes the more
polyvalent aspects of language, such as metaphor, and is the source of insight,
but, crucially, it does not speak. This part of the mind isn't directly
accessible but it can be influenced through intention, such as setting your
interior alarm clock in order to wake up at a certain time in the morning. But
it does far more: it is the place where connections are made, where our most
complex thinking is done. Its perceptions are alive. We can open to this hidden
treasure by what is often called unknowing,
a kenotic relinquishing of the self-conscious mind's ideas, concepts,
expectations and speech, along with the two-dimensional analytic faculty that
generates them.
0 Comments:
Post a Comment
<< Home