III Apophatic Prayer as a Theological Model...
The Paradox of Intention and the Subversion of
Self-Consciousness
In the late 20th century there have emerged
a number of new terms that do not exist in ancient languages, such as
‘self-consciousness’, by which I mean in this paper what is expressed by the phrase,
‘He is so self-conscious it is embarrassing’, or ‘she made me
self-conscious’. Although a
consistent and specific terminology for self-consciousness and its suspension
was absent until the late 19th century, the perception of these phenomena were present. In this discussion, by the suspension
of self-consciousness I mean that phenomenon that is part of the spatial
continuum of ordinary consciousness, and which is most easily observed in
children but which also occurs in adults, when concentration becomes so
complete that a distraction causes them to realise that they have ‘returned to
themselves’.
A phrase that describes the dynamic of
self-consciousness and its suspension, ‘the paradox of intention’, has existed
only since 1988, when Marvin Shaw published his eponymous book. This is one of the most fundamental
laws by which self-consciousness operates, and it is implicit in many ancient
texts.[i] Because the writers of these texts do
not have a universal language for self-consciousness and the paradox of
intention, which appear to be universal constants in the human psyche not
subject to time and culture in the way language is, the language they used for
these phenomena is neither common nor consistent. As they struggle to describe phenomena for which there
are yet few words, their texts are culturally vulnerable and easily
misinterpreted.[ii] Before giving examples, it is necessary
briefly to describe the paradox of intention.
The paradox of intention is a simple
descriptor for a complex and familiar process that to a large extent governs
the way humans think. It is very
difficult, if not impossible, to think about anything but the simplest objects
directly.[iii] Instead, the mind works by description,
attribution of functions, and so forth.
The most common manifestation of the paradox of intention is that
experience of having a name, a reference, on the tip of the tongue, and being
unable to express it. The greater
the effort to remember, the more self-consciousness intrudes and the
sought-after information retreats.
To recover the lost information, it is
necessary to forget the object of remembering, that is to say, it is necessary
to subvert self-consciousness, which is casting an ironic eye and making
unhelpful interior remarks such as, ‘What a dork you are for not being able to
remember this’. The forgetting
cannot be a half-forgetting, with one eye on the imaginary place where the
misplaced information may re-emerge;
the shift of concentration must be complete. This phenomenon extends to many areas of life:
There is a universal and recurrent human experience in
which the blocking of some sought for attainment leads us to modify our
intention, and this is found to yield an unexpected fulfillment of its
own; experiences of impossibility
force us to abandon our pretensions and our intensity, and in this we
surprisingly achieve either what we sought or a kind of contentment that we
thought could only follow the conquest of that which blocked us. In either case we discover that the
goal is reached by giving up the attempt to reach it.[iv]
[i] (Atlanta, Scholar’s
Press, 1988). Shaw’s survey covers
sayings in a variety of religions, but he seems to stop short of making the
correlation attempted here. The
paradox of intention is pervasive, as he notes. Beyond the example given, its effects are subtle and wide-ranging,
although it is essential to retain a distinction between intention, attention
and performance, and the communication between conscious and what is unseen in
the mind, which includes far more than the ‘unconscious’. Weight-loss provides a good example of
the distinction between intention and performance: weight will not be lost by eating more -- this is a
confusion of performance with intention.
But to have the intention of losing weight as a deep desire, and to
consciously intend a different and more encompassing aspiration, such as
mountain-climbing, whose performance necessitates weight-loss and which
intention entails it, may effect the desired result (it should be noted that
the paradox of intention by definition does not guarantee any result). In other words, weight-loss is no
longer a self-reflective stricture, but part of the natural flow of
self-forgetful aspiration towards a higher goal.
The application to issues of
morality and ethics is self evident: taught as a set of rigid rules to be imposed as a
template will inspire only rebellion as Paul notes above; while as part of an
all-encompassing and passionately desired higher goal, a moral and ethical
life, while not without effort, slips naturally into place. The key word is an old-fashioned
one: integrity. That is, the truth of the self, the
operation of the divine in the uniqueness of each person needs to have the
freedom and reverence to unfold.
This highest aspiration entails all else and focuses the intention away
from the subject. By contrast, the
psychobabble that surrounds ‘self-esteem’ is a poor substutite , more
significantly, self-defeating as well because it is reflexive. As shall be seen, morality entailed by
the highest aspiration according the model presented is the opposite of
individualism, which can lead only to facism.
[ii] Isaac of Nineveh (7th
c) offers what is perhaps the most consistent language and modeling of the mind
and its relationship to the body, which may be why his Ascetical Homilies
for centuries were the only text given to novice monks in the Orthodox
churches. See The Ascetical
Homlies of Isaac the Syrian, tr. Dana Miller, Holy Transfiguration
Monastery, Boston, 1984, p. 297.
Hereafter cited as A.H. The
translation by Wensinck may be more readily available: Mystic Treatises by Isaac of Nineveh,
tr. A.J. Wensinck, Nieuwe Reeks, Deel XXIII, No. 1, Wiesbaden, 1969. Hereafter cited as Bedjan, as references are to Bedjan’s numbering in
the margin of Wensinck’s translation.
The phrase ‘their mind was snatched’ occurs in Bedjan 171.
[iii] One of the goals of
Zen meditation is to see objects directly.
[iv] Shaw, p. 195.
3 Comments:
Have you read "The origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bi-Cameral Mind" by Julian Jaynes? It is fron the 70s but is now making a comeback. The first fe chapters are about this and fascinating to say the least. It was a life-changing read for me in 1978, and every two or three years I read it again. I am presently re-reading for the umpteenth time right now. I do think you might in-joy it.
Joel, thanks. Two days ago someone mentioned it to me and it will be in my pile of books at Bodley on Monday.
McGilchrist (p. 261) mentions it as a very early parallel to what he is doing—but that while there is much to admire, Jaynes came to opposite conclusions. Jaynes was coming from a psychoanalytic position; McGilchrist is coming from the laboratory. As I understand it (having read only McGilchrist and not yet Jaynes), Jayes thought the two hemispheres operated entirely separately, while McGilchrist's book shows that research has shown that both hemispheres are involved in everything but that in certain tasks one hemisphere dominates over the other. Interestingly, the function of the corpus callosum is inhibitory....
Post a Comment
<< Home